Although the voice in a free field has an excellent recruitment by a cochlear implant (CI), the situation is different for music because it is a much more complex process, where perceiving the pitch discrimination becomes important to appreciate it. The aim of this study is to determine the music perception abilities among children with Cis and to verify the benefit of a training period for specific musical frequency discrimination.
Scheda Allenamento Boxe Pdf To Wordpress
Our main goals were to prepare a computer tool for pitch discrimination training and to assess musical improvements. Ten children, aged between 5 and 12 years, with optimal phoneme recognition in quiet and with no disabilities associated with deafness, were selected to join the training. Each patient received, before training period, two types of exams: a pitch discrimination test, consisting of discovering if two notes were different or not; and a music test consisting of two identification tasks (melodic and full version) of one musicitem among 5 popular childhood songs.
After assessment, a music training software was designed and utilised individually at home for a period of six months. The results following complete training showed significantly higher performance in the task of frequency discrimination. After a proper musical training identification, frequency discrimination performance was significantly higher (p. IntroductionIn recent years, advancements in cochlear implant (CI) technology has allowed the device to achieve its primary goal, that is to restore a near-normal speech understanding in profoundly deaf subjects, at least in favourable listening conditions.
New signal processing strategies have brought benefits for CI users in terms of perception. Technological innovations in CI systems, enabled functional hearing, oral speech and language achievements in many children with pre-lingual severe-to-profound hearing impairment.Nonetheless, CIs still provide poorer auditory information than those conveyed through an intact natural cochlea. One of the main weaknesses of the latest generation CIs appears to be the limited number of active channels, too low to allow an appropriate encoding of detailed spectral information, which is crucial to give perceptual accuracy of melody pitch patterns. Moreover, most of the current processing strategies remove fine temporal structure information from stimulus waveforms, therefore limiting the users' ability to extract pitch cues from temporal components of the signal. In fact, CI encoding signal algorithms generally fail in reproducing higher order harmonics. Preservation of tonotopicity is crucial in CI-mediated music understanding, mainly because it demands an absolute fidelity of signal transduction, while there is mounting evidence that the tonotopic representation of frequencies through the CI is often distorted due to a poor correspondence between the frequency bands allocated to the electrodes, according to the conventional frequency maps, and the pitch elicited by stimulation of the same electrodes. Thus, fundamental frequencies – and consequently harmonics – cannot be efficiently extracted and properly decoded, due to the mismatch between electrode- assigned frequencies and pitch -.
Distribution of frequency bands of the strategy encodes. Histogram shows the distribution of frequency bandwidths on each electrode, which is in proportion to the cochlear tonotopicity following the placement of the electrodes. The second curve represents the frequency range transduced by the implant according to the distribution of the bands set in the histogram. Data were extrapolated from the stimulation of the mapping software of the processor for each patient. In this case, it shows the frequency distribution of patient C1.It is widely accepted that music and speech are the most complex sound features produced by the human species. These two processes have similar properties and central processing, albeit analysed in different human brain areas. The inter-hemispheric domains of music and speech allow psychosocial and cognitive skills development in communication.
The 'musician effect' persists under degraded pitch condition of CI simulation and may offer advantages in pitch processing. Studies with normal-hearing people showed that musical training can improve pitch recognition: musician children detect pitch variations in both music and language much more accurately - and rapidly than non-musician peers. Although the voice in a free field has an excellent recruitment by the CI, the situation is different concerning musical sounds because it is a much more complex process, where perceiving the higher order harmonics becomes important to appreciate it.
Many studies have shown that some CI recipients are quite able to perceive features such as tempo and rhythm, but the extent of this perception is variable. Other papers have focused on pitch processing skills due to their immediate relevance to music perception -. In some of these studies, familiar melodies have been recognised from a closed set, whereas in others simple melodic contours have been discriminated. The results indicate that melody perception is generally poor in CI users, again considering a large inter-individual variability. Several studies have examined the accuracy of different devices and coding strategies on melody and speech in noise perception, concluding that there are statistically significant correlations between pitch ranking and familiar melody recognition. Children with CIs have greater difficulties in recognising familiar songs when these melodies are without words. Recent research demonstrated that there is a correlation between music perception and phonological and reading process skills; the same authors hypothesised that some music characteristics (such as rhythm and pitch) are associated with some speech perception parameters.
However, standardised methods for assessing music perception in patients with CI are lacking. Found that musical training is an effective rehabilitation tool for auditory perception improvement.
In particular, one of the most relevant points of this work is the ability to subject children to this training without requiring them any additional efforts, thanks to the possibility to perform the exercises at patient's home, by themselves or with the help of a family member if the subjects were too young. The authors stated that submitting children to musical exercises – such as pitch discrimination test between two notes – leads to improvements in spoken language perception. Dastgheib proposed a new music training program based on language development to optimise speech and language skills. These findings show that the CI alone does not satisfy all patients' needs, and that speech therapy and specific training may be proper and necessary in order to maximise CI benefits.The purpose of the present investigation is to determine whether children with CIs can benefit from training on pitch and music perception in terms of pitch discrimination; moreover, if pitch perception can be trained, it could also lead to improvements in speech perception and in music enjoyment. SubjectsTen children (6 boys and 4 girls), monaurally Nucleus™ CI users , who periodically came to our ENT clinic to perform speech processor fitting, were recruited. Children were aged between 5 and 12 years and had no disability associated with deafness.
Mean chronological age was 117 ± 36 months, while mean hearing age (i.e. Months of CI use) was 77 ± 26 months. These subjects had bilateral hearing loss and regularly used conventional hearing aids until CI implantation.
All these patients had been using a Nucleus™ CI device for at least 6 months: 5 had a perimodiolar electrode (CI24RE-CA) and 5 had a straight, non-perimodiolar electrode (CI24R-S). At the time the study took place, 8 of the 10 Nucleus™ CI recipients were using a Freedom™ speech processor and 2 were fitted with an ESPrit 3G™ speech processor.
Six had been using an ACE™ strategy with a 900 pps stimulation rate and 25 μs pulse width, 3 patients had been using an ACE™ (RE) strategy with a 2400 pps stimulation rate and 12 μs pulse width and the last one using SPEAK strategy with a 250 pps and 25 μs. During the month preceding the test session, all speech processors were fitted so that all patients could receive comfortable stimulation. Impedance measurement and neural response telemetry (NRT™ for Nucleus™ recipients) were performed for all electrodes in each subject. As soon as it was possible, all subjects were enrolled in the auditory-musical training program of the Catholic University of Sacred Heart in Rome. None of the patients had been attending music classes at school, nor they had been taking part in any formal music training activity, so that they belonged to musical experience level '1' according to Looi.
Before introducing our test battery to CI subjects, we checked its validity on 10 normal hearing patients, all of them scoring between 95 and 100% in the Music Training Program based on Musical Pitch Discrimination (MPD) test. SubjectAgeBackground informationDeafnessSide of CIAetiologyOnset ageDuration of ciC 112Idiopathic48Pre-verbalRightC 26Idiopathic24Pre-verbalRightC 311Waardenburg Syndrome29Pre-verbalLeftC 48Heterozygous for connexin 26 mutation35Pre-verbalRightC 512Idiopathic39Pre-verbalRightC 612Idiopathic48Pre-verbalRightC 712Idiopathic66Post-verbalRightC 811Idiopathic47Pre-verbalRightC 95Heterozygous for connexin 26 mutation23Pre-verbalRightC 106Idiopathic24Pre-verbalRight. SubjectDevice- featuresTotal frequencyProcessorImplantMaximaChannel stimolation rateStrategyPulse widthC 1Freedom SPCI24R (CS)8900ACE257200C 2Freedom SPCI24RE (CA)102400ACE (RE)1224000C 3Freedom SPCI24R (CS)8250SPEAK252000C 4Freedom SPCI24RE (CA)8900ACE257200C 5Freedom SPCI24R (CS)8900ACE257200C 6ESPrit 3GCI24R (CS)8900ACE257200C 7Freedom SPCI24RE (CA)102400ACE (RE)1224000C 8ESPrit 3GCI24R (CS)8900ACE257200C 9Freedom SPCI24RE (CA)8900ACE257200C 10Freedom SPCI24RE (CA)102400ACE (RE)127200. Music test batteryA music test battery was designed in order to assess CImediated perception of music. It included a Music Training Software based on MPD, and a Music Test. In the pitch discrimination test, stimuli consisted in pairs of notes played by a piano and distanced by at least one semitone (approximately 6% F0 difference), being the semitone the smallest interval size in traditional Western music. The notes were distributed within the three central octaves (C4, C5, C6, each matching with the following frequency bands: 262 Hz-523 Hz, 523 Hz-1046 Hz and 1046 Hz- 1976 Hz), used for most of the songs, for a total of 36 notes.
After listening to each pair of notes, the patient was asked to indicate which one was higher in pitch.Music training software. This software is designed to reproduce the melodic exercises of musical pitch discrimination: patients can perform exercises at home as shown in the present study. The Home-Learning Program is composed of several interfaces and levels of increasing difficulty. According to the test, subjects listen to 2 musical notes, and then say if the pair is made of the same or different sounds. The notes were played by a piano and, as in the previous test, lay between the 4 th and 6 th octave. Each pair was then recorded (sampling at 40 KHz), reproduced from the Home-Learning Program and delivered to the listener, sitting one meter away from a loudspeaker, sending a stimulus at 70 dB nHL.
The software Home-Learning Program was installed on an IBM™ computer, which was routinely used to fit patients' maps in routine fitting sessions. The levels were 7, with increasing difficulty, determined by reducing the distance between notes: the 1 st level comprised pairs of notes 12-semitones apart (easiest task), while the 7 th level included notes one semitone apart (most difficult task; ).
The test aimed to assess children's frequency discrimination in the frequency domain 262-1976 Hz. For each level, one pair of musical notes was administered: children had to say if the two tones were different or the same. Each level is further divided into two tests.TEST A: composed of two parts (each of 10 questions) in which the subject had to choose between 2 notes belonging to the 5 th and 6 th musical octave (523-1976 Hz).TEST B: consists of two parts (each of 10 questions) in which the subject had to choose between 2 notes belonging to the 4 th and 5 th musical octave (262- 988 Hz).In the last 2 levels (6 th and 7 th level) there are 20 more questions test that investigate discrimination on mid-range (linked to the 5 th octave). The software was programmed to go to the next, harder level once 8 correct answers of 10 were obtained. The threshold of 8 correct answers was planned conforming to the Theorem Bernoulli Trials. The Home-Learning Program was given to study members' families; they were asked to set aside time (at least 2 hours weekly) to practice at home for a 6-month period.
Parents had to teach children how to use the software.Music Test. The music test aims to assess children's identification skills in a closed set of music items. It is composed of 5 childhood songs in digital recording, synthesised with Finale™ 2008 (MakeMusic Inc., Eden Prairie, MN).
The tunes were presented at 70 dBnHL, coming from two frontal loudspeakers one metre apart from the CI recipient. Prior to test execution, subjects were conditioned to look at a specific cartoon movie linked to each song from an IBM laptop © (IBM, Armonk, USA).
Each song is presented twice, for a total of 10 items. During testing, children sat in front of a screen showing the most representative characters for each of the previously seen cartoons. The music test is divided in two sessions: in session 1, full version songs (instrumental plus vocal), as those presented in the preliminary training, were played twice randomly, for a total of 10 items. Children had to indicate the distinctive character on the screen for each song. In session 2, children were exposed to the melodic version songs, presented twice in a random order for a total of 10 musical items. An overall score was calculated on the basis of the items correctly identified.All children carried out an initial test to define the baseline music performances (musical pitch discrimination + music test). They then followed the specific musical training program, and eventually performed a final test with the same initial workup to evaluate possible improvement due to training.
Statistical analysisWe used a Kolmogorov-Smirnov test (K-S test) to compare a sample with a reference probability distribution, a paired t-Student statistic test to determine whether there were differences between two means or between a target value and a calculated mean, and used the Mann-Whitney U test, a non-parametric statistical test, when the distribution of samples did not respect of K-S test condition. A linear regression model according to Spearman's rank and Pearson's coefficient was used for correlations.
Significance was set at p. MPD resultsScores achieved after musical training showed significantly higher performance in frequency discrimination tasks than before training.
At the baseline assessment, 2 children reached level 1 (frequency discrimination threshold of 12 semitones), 3 children reached level 2 (frequency discrimination threshold of 10 semitones), 4 children level 3 (frequency discrimination threshold of 8 semitones) and 1 child got to level 5 (frequency discrimination threshold of 4 semitones). Conversely, at the final assessment, 1 children came up to level 4, 1 to level 5, 5 children to level 6 (frequency discrimination threshold of 2 semitones) and the remaining 3 children got to the 7 th and final level (frequency discrimination threshold of 1 semitone). Patients who reached last and hardest discrimination level (P3, P5 and P6) showed a proportional improvement in melody test scores : this suggests that these patients have higher frequency resolution than other CI children, but not comparable to that of normal hearing peers. In fact, children in the control group performed the MPD test without committing any error.
The results showed that all children improved after training, each having reached more advanced level (almost up to the highest level). Statistical analysis was performed to rule out bias such as chance level; the Box-Plot and histogram show the significant difference in performances (p. DiscussionMusic is a challenging task that is generally more difficult than conventional speech.
Music is also a powerful tool in auditory training in children with CIs because it is an integral part of human natural environment. Music perception by CI recipients is hard because most common signal-processing strategies fail in transmitting effective pitch information. ConclusionsAt present, however, no one can truly know how CI users with preverbal deafness perceive the musical melody. There is no doubt that as long as we do not find an effective solution to the lack of frequency discrimination and mismatch, these patients may not appreciate the beauty of music, even if they are able to recognise different songs. Nowadays, no codified procedure for a standardised music assessment nor specific musical training is available, especially for hearing impaired children.In this paper, we present a completely new tool to train CI children in pitch discrimination and melody identification tasks.
We found a significant difference between pre- and post-training scores in the full version test (p = 0.0071), melodic test (p = 0.0151) and frequency discrimination test (0.0001). In the light of our findings, we can conclude that is possible to achieve improvements in frequency discrimination and song recognition following specific perceptual training in prelingually deafened CI children.